Friday, January 27, 2012

Build Your Cognitive Reserve

 sharpbrains » JUL 23, 2007

Build Your Cognitive Reserve-Yaakov Stern
By: Alvaro Fernandez

Dr. Yaakov Stern is the Divi­sion Leader of the Cog­ni­tive Neu­ro­science Divi­sion of the Sergievsky Cen­ter, and Pro­fes­sor of Clin­i­cal Neu­ropsy­chol­ogy, at the Col­lege of Physi­cians and Sur­geons of Colum­bia Uni­ver­sity, New York.
 He is one of the lead­ing pro­po­nents of the Cog­ni­tive reserve the­ory, which aims to explain why some indi­vid­u­als with full Alzheimer’s pathol­ogy (accu­mu­la­tion of plaques and tan­gles in their brains) can keep nor­mal lives until they die, while oth­ers –with the same amount of plaques and tan­gles– dis­play the severe symp­toms we asso­ciate with Alzheimer’s Dis­ease.
He has pub­lished dozens of peer-reviewed sci­en­tific papers on the subject.

 The con­cept of a Cog­ni­tive Reserve has been around since 1989, when a post mortem analy­sis of 137 peo­ple with Alzheimer’s Dis­ease showed that some patients exhib­ited fewer clin­i­cal symp­toms than their actual pathol­ogy sug­tested.
These patients also showed higher brain weights and greater num­ber of neu­rons when com­pared to age-matched con­trolls.
The inves­ti­ga­tors hypoth­e­sized that the patients had a larger “reserve” of neu­rons and abil­i­ties that enable them to off­set the losses caused by Alzheimer’s.
Since then, the con­cept of Cog­ni­tive Reserve has been defined as the abil­ity of an indi­vid­ual to tol­er­ate pro­gres­sive brain pathol­ogy with­out demon­strat­ing clin­i­cal cog­ni­tive symp­toms.

 Key take-aways -
Life­time expe­ri­ences, like edu­ca­tion, engag­ing occu­pa­tion, and leisure activ­i­ties, have been shown to have a major influ­ence on how we age, specif­i­cally on whether we will develop Alzheimer’s symp­toms or not.
This is so because stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties, ide­ally com­bin­ing phys­i­cal exer­cise, learn­ing and social inter­ac­tion, help us build a Cog­ni­tive Reserve to pro­tect us.
The ear­lier we start build­ing our Reserve, the bet­ter; but it is never too late to start.
And, the more activ­i­ties, the bet­ter: the effect is cumulative.

The Cog­ni­tive Reserve
 Alvaro Fer­nan­dez (AF): Dear Dr. Stern, it is a plea­sure to have you here. Let me first ask you this: the impli­ca­tions of your research are pretty astound­ing, pre­sent­ing major impli­ca­tions across sec­tors and age groups. What has been the most unex­pected reac­tion so far?

 YS: well…I was pretty sur­prised when, years ago, a reporter from Sev­en­teen mag­a­zine requested an inter­view. I was really curi­ous to learn why she felt that her read­ers would be inter­ested in stud­ies about demen­tia. What she told me showed a deep under­stand­ing and insight: she wanted to moti­vate chil­dren to stay in school. She under­stood that early social inter­ven­tions could be very pow­er­ful for build­ing reserve and pre­vent­ing dementia.

 AF: That’s great…so let’s now fast for­ward, say, 60 years from our high-school years, and sup­pose that per­sons A and B both tech­ni­cally have Alzheimer’s (plaques and tan­gles appear in the brain), but only A is show­ing the dis­ease symp­toms. What may explain this discrepancy?

 YS: Indi­vid­u­als who lead men­tally stim­u­lat­ing lives, through edu­ca­tion, occu­pa­tion and leisure activ­i­ties, have reduced risk of devel­op­ing Alzheimer’s. Stud­ies sug­gest that they have 35–40% less risk of man­i­fest­ing the dis­ease. The pathol­ogy will still occur, but they are able to cope with it bet­ter. Some won’t ever be diag­nosed with Alzheimer’s because they don’t present any symp­toms. In stud­ies that fol­low healthy elders over time and then get autop­sies, up to 20% of peo­ple who did not present any sig­nif­i­cant prob­lem in the daily lives have full blown Alzheimer’s pathol­ogy in their brains.

 AF: What exactly may be going on in the brain that pro­vides that level of protection?

 YS: There are two ideas that are com­ple­men­tary. One idea (called Brain Reserve by researchers) pos­tu­lates that some indi­vid­u­als have a greater num­ber of neu­rons and synapses, and that some­how those extra struc­tures pro­vide a level of pro­tec­tion. In a sense, we have more “hard­ware”, pro­vid­ing a pas­sive pro­tec­tion against the attacks of Alzheimer’s.
The other the­ory (called Cog­ni­tive Reserve) empha­sizes the build­ing of new capa­bil­i­ties, how peo­ple can per­form tasks bet­ter through prac­tice, and how these skills become so well learned that they are not too easy to unlearn. Like devel­op­ing new and refined “software”.

 AF: But, both seem to go hand in hand, cor­rect? Neu­ro­plas­tic­ity means that what you call “hard­ware” and “soft­ware” are two sides of the same coin and they influ­ence each other, right?

 YS: Cor­rect. So these days we don’t make a sharp dis­tinc­tion, and are con­duct­ing more neu­roimag­ing stud­ies to bet­ter under­stand the rela­tion­ship between both.

 Build­ing Your Cog­ni­tive Reserve

 AF: OK, so our goal is to build that Reserve of neu­rons, synapses, and skills. How can we do that? What defines “men­tally stim­u­lat­ing activ­i­ties” or good “brain exercise”?

 YS: In sum­mary, we could say that “stim­u­la­tion” con­sists of engag­ing in activ­i­ties. In our research almost all activ­i­ties are seen to con­tribute to reserve. Some have chal­leng­ing lev­els of cog­ni­tive com­plex­ity, and some have inter­per­sonal or phys­i­cal demands.
In ani­mal stud­ies, expo­sure to an enriched envi­ron­ment or increased phys­i­cal activ­ity result in increased neu­ro­ge­n­e­sis (the cre­ation of new neu­rons).
You can get that stim­u­la­tion through edu­ca­tion and/ or your occu­pa­tion. There is clear research show­ing how those two ele­ments reduce the risk.
Now, what is very excit­ing is that, no mat­ter one’s age, edu­ca­tion and occu­pa­tion, our level of par­tic­i­pa­tion in leisure activ­i­ties has a sig­nif­i­cant and cumu­la­tive effect.
A key mes­sage here is that dif­fer­ent activ­i­ties have inde­pen­dent, syn­er­gis­tic, con­tri­bu­tions, which means the more things you do and the ear­lier you start, the bet­ter.
But you are never stuck: bet­ter late than never.

 AF: Can you give us some exam­ples of those leisure activ­i­ties that seem to have the most pos­i­tive effects?
 YS: For our 2001 study we eval­u­ated the effect of 13 activ­i­ties, com­bin­ing intel­lec­tual, phys­i­cal, and social ele­menus.
Some of the activ­i­ties with the most effect were read­ing, vis­it­ing friends or rel­a­tives, going to movies or restau­rants, and walk­ing for plea­sure or going on an excur­sion.
As you can see, a vari­ety. We saw that the group with high level of leisure activ­i­ties pre­sented 38% less risk (con­trol­ling for other fac­tors) of devel­op­ing Alzheimer’s symp­toms.
And that, for each addi­tional type of activ­ity, the risk got reduced by 8%.
There is an addi­tional ele­ment that we are start­ing to see more clearly.
Phys­i­cal exer­cise, by itself, also has a very ben­e­fi­cial impact on cog­ni­tion. Only a few months ago researchers were able to show for the first time how phys­i­cal activ­ity pro­motes neu­ro­ge­n­e­sis in the human brain. So, we need both men­tal and phys­i­cal exer­cise.
The not-so-good news is that, as of today, there no clear recipe for suc­cess. More research is needed before we pre­pare a sys­tem­atic set of inter­ven­tions that can help max­i­mize our protection.

 AF: We typ­i­cally empha­size the impor­tance of a good nutri­tion, phys­i­cal exer­cise, stress man­age­ment and men­tal exer­cise that presents nov­elty, vari­ety and chal­lenge. What do you think of the rel­a­tively recent appear­ance of so many computer-based cog­ni­tive train­ing pro­grams, some more science-based than others?

 YS: Those ele­ments you men­tion make sense. The prob­lem is that, at least from the point of view of Alzheimer’s, we can­not be much more spe­cific. We don’t know if learn­ing a new lan­guage is more ben­e­fi­cial than learn­ing a new musi­cal instru­ment or using a computer-based pro­gram. A few of the cog­ni­tive train­ing com­puter pro­grams we have seen, like the one you dis­cussed with Prof. Daniel Gopher to train the men­tal abil­i­ties of pilots, seem to have clear effects on cog­ni­tion, gen­er­al­iz­ing beyond the train­ing itself. But, for the most part, it is too early to tell the long-term effects. We need bet­ter designed clin­i­cal tri­als with clear con­trols. Right now, the most we can say is that those who lead men­tally stim­u­lat­ing lives, through edu­ca­tion, occu­pa­tion and leisure activ­i­ties seem to have the least risk of devel­op­ing Alzheimer’s Disease.

 Research inter­ests
 AF: Tell us know a bit more about your cur­rent research
 YS: We are study­ing a num­ber of related areas, apply­ing neu­roimag­ing tech­niques to under­stand how exactly all these Cog­ni­tive Reserve con­cepts are imple­mented in the brain. One, we want to under­stand indi­vid­ual dif­fer­ences in how peo­ple approach tasks. We want to mea­sure their effi­ciency and capac­ity the brain net­works that medi­ate tasks per­for­mance with the idea that those with greater effi­ciency and capac­ity might cope bet­ter with age-related prob­lems.
For exam­ple, we can all under­stand that a com­pet­i­tive swim­mer is going to swim bet­ter than I would even if he has some weights in his legs, but we haven’t yet iden­ti­fied what exactly is the equiv­a­lent in the brain. Sec­ond, we want to under­stand how old peo­ple com­pen­sate for the areas of decline.
For exam­ple, do they begin to use new brain areas when the ones that are typ­i­cally used start to fail. Third, whether the Cog­ni­tive Reserve presents ben­e­fits beyond the pre­ven­tion of Alzheimer’s symp­toms. Does hav­ing a higher reserve result in bet­ter atten­tion, bet­ter exec­u­tive func­tions, more suc­cess­ful aging overall?

 AF: All very impor­tant top­ics. And I am sure every­one read­ing this inter­view will devour any new details on how to build our Cog­ni­tive Reserves. Thank you for your time, and please keep us informed.

 YS: My plea­sure. Thank you for your great edu­ca­tional initiative. ————————————

No comments: